
conscious, slow and error-prone and ‘automatic’ processes (e.g., writing one’sname),
which are regarded as being unintentional, effortless, involuntary, unconscious and fast
(e.g., see Shiffrin and Schneider, 1977). In an effort to account for certain subsequent
empirical anomalies (e.g., the fact that some automatic processes are amenable to
consciousness and can be controlled intentionally whereas others are not; Sternberg
and Sternberg, 2009), ‘dual process’ views of attention (e.g., Evans and Stanovich,
2013) have given way to a more fine-grained analysis of this construct. For example, a
number of researchers have proposed hybrid theories of skilled action which have
sought to capture the synthesis between cognitive and automatic processes (e.g.,
Mylopoulos and Pacherie, 2017).
And yet, despite the emergence of these latter perspectives, dual process views of
skill learning (which place cognitive processes on a continuum ranging from fully
controlled to fully automatic) continue to hold sway in the skill acquisition/sport
psychology literature (e.g., Furley et al., 2015). Before considering the utility of hybrid
theories, however, we should provide a brief overview of the dual process view and
consider how this perspective has shaped current conceptualisations of skilled action.
Dual process views typically portray learning as involving a gradual reduction of
cognitive resources as one acquires an increasing level of control over motor execution.
Take, for example, Fitts and Posner’s(1967) highly influential model of skill acquisi-
tion which postulates that learning occurs in three linear stages. In the initial cognitive
stage, agents draw on explicit rules to consciously guide task performance. This will
inevitably require them to experiment with different strategies to see which brings them
closer to a movement goal (Wulf, 2007). The associative stage is characterised by
increasingly consistent and economical movement patterns. In a final autonomous
stage, movement production is fluent, effortless, immune to dual task interference,
and requires little cognitive control. Similar to a number of other influential models of
skill learning (e.g., Anderson’s, 1982, Adaptive Control of Thought Theory), Fitts and
Posner theorise that controlled and automatic processes reside at opposite ends of a
single continuum. According to this view, low automaticity is associated with high
levels of control and vice versa.
Another feature common to dual process views is the belief that automaticity is a
defining feature of optimal performance while control is associated with the degrada-
tion of motor execution (Masters and Maxwell, 2008). Advocates of this perspective
often present the phenomenon of “expert induced amnesia” (when asked to recall how
they have performed a task experts provide impoverished accounts containing little
recollection of the episodic ‘rules’ that guided their action; see Beilock et al. 2002)as
prima facie evidence that skilled actions are governed by automatic processes. In a
number of experimental studies, skilled performers have reported few explicit episodic
rules relating to the execution of a highly practiced motor skill (e.g., Beilock et al.
2002). Unfortunately, the experimental paradigm adopted by many of these studies
presents performers with conditions that are static and largely unchanging (take the
standard putting task which is such a popular means of testing motor control in
laboratory studies; see Christensen et al., 2015, for a detailed critique). As a result,
the tasks are hampered by a lack of real-world complexity – key features that one
would find in a naturalistic setting (e.g., changes in environmental conditions or high
levels of performance pressure). It is perhaps unsurprising that ‘experts’ in Beilock’s
study appeared amnesiac when asked to report what they had focused on when
Exploring the Orthogonal Relationship between Controlled and...